skip to content
Turn-Taking in Human Communicative Interaction Preview this item
ClosePreview this item
Checking...

Turn-Taking in Human Communicative Interaction

Author: Stephen Levinson C; Judith Holler; Kobin Kendrick H; Marisa Casillas
Publisher: s.l. Frontiers Media SA 2016
Edition/Format:   eBook : DocumentView all editions and formats
Summary:
The core use of language is in face-to-face conversation. This is characterized by rapid turn-taking. This turn-taking poses a number central puzzles for the psychology of language. Consider, for example, that in large corpora the gap between turns is on the order of 100 to 300 ms, but the latencies involved in language production require minimally between 600ms (for a single word) or 1500 ms (for as simple  Read more...
Rating:

(not yet rated) 0 with reviews - Be the first.

Subjects
More like this

 

Find a copy online

Links to this item

Find a copy in the library

&AllPage.SpinnerRetrieving; Finding libraries that hold this item...

Details

Material Type: Document, Internet resource
Document Type: Internet Resource, Computer File
All Authors / Contributors: Stephen Levinson C; Judith Holler; Kobin Kendrick H; Marisa Casillas
ISBN: 9782889198252 2889198251
OCLC Number: 960769616
Language Note: English
Description: 1 Online-Ressource (1 electronic resource ( p.))

Abstract:

The core use of language is in face-to-face conversation. This is characterized by rapid turn-taking. This turn-taking poses a number central puzzles for the psychology of language. Consider, for example, that in large corpora the gap between turns is on the order of 100 to 300 ms, but the latencies involved in language production require minimally between 600ms (for a single word) or 1500 ms (for as simple sentence). This implies that participants in conversation are predicting the ends of the incoming turn and preparing in advance. But how is this done? What aspects of this prediction are done when? What happens when the prediction is wrong? What stops participants coming in too early? If the system is running on prediction, why is there consistently a mode of 100 to 300 ms in response time? The timing puzzle raises further puzzles: it seems that comprehension must run parallel with the preparation for production, but it has been presumed that there are strict cognitive limitations on more than one central process running at a time. How is this bottleneck overcome? Far from being 'easy' as some psychologists have suggested, conversation may be one of the most demanding cognitive tasks in our everyday lives. Further questions naturally arise: how do children learn to master this demanding task, and what is the developmental trajectory in this domain? Research shows that aspects of turn-taking such as its timing are remarkably stable across languages and cultures, but the word order of languages varies enormously. How then does prediction of the incoming turn work when the verb (often the informational nugget in a clause) is at the end? Conversely, how can production work fast enough in languages that have the verb at the beginning, thereby requiring early planning of the whole clause? What happens when one changes modality, as in sign languages -- with the loss of channel constraints is turn-taking much freer? And what about face-to-face communication amongst hearing individuals -- do gestures, gaze, and other body behaviors facilitate turn-taking? One can also ask the phylogenetic question: how did such a system evolve? There seem to be parallels (analogies) in duetting bird species, and in a variety of monkey species, but there is little evidence of anything like this among the great apes. All this constitutes a neglected set of problems at the heart of the psychology of language and of the language sciences. This research topic welcomes contributions f ...

Reviews

User-contributed reviews
Retrieving GoodReads reviews...
Retrieving DOGObooks reviews...

Tags

Be the first.

Similar Items

Related Subjects:(2)

Confirm this request

You may have already requested this item. Please select Ok if you would like to proceed with this request anyway.

Linked Data


Primary Entity

<http://www.worldcat.org/oclc/960769616> # Turn-Taking in Human Communicative Interaction
    a schema:Book, schema:CreativeWork, schema:MediaObject ;
    library:oclcnum "960769616" ;
    library:placeOfPublication <http://experiment.worldcat.org/entity/work/data/4189268028#Place/s_l> ; # s.l.
    schema:about <http://experiment.worldcat.org/entity/work/data/4189268028#Topic/psychology> ; # Psychology
    schema:author <http://experiment.worldcat.org/entity/work/data/4189268028#Person/kendrick_h_kobin> ; # Kobin Kendrick H.
    schema:author <http://experiment.worldcat.org/entity/work/data/4189268028#Person/holler_judith> ; # Judith Holler
    schema:author <http://experiment.worldcat.org/entity/work/data/4189268028#Person/levinson_c_stephen> ; # Stephen Levinson C.
    schema:author <http://experiment.worldcat.org/entity/work/data/4189268028#Person/casillas_marisa> ; # Marisa Casillas
    schema:bookFormat schema:EBook ;
    schema:datePublished "2016" ;
    schema:description "The core use of language is in face-to-face conversation. This is characterized by rapid turn-taking. This turn-taking poses a number central puzzles for the psychology of language. Consider, for example, that in large corpora the gap between turns is on the order of 100 to 300 ms, but the latencies involved in language production require minimally between 600ms (for a single word) or 1500 ms (for as simple sentence). This implies that participants in conversation are predicting the ends of the incoming turn and preparing in advance. But how is this done? What aspects of this prediction are done when? What happens when the prediction is wrong? What stops participants coming in too early? If the system is running on prediction, why is there consistently a mode of 100 to 300 ms in response time? The timing puzzle raises further puzzles: it seems that comprehension must run parallel with the preparation for production, but it has been presumed that there are strict cognitive limitations on more than one central process running at a time. How is this bottleneck overcome? Far from being 'easy' as some psychologists have suggested, conversation may be one of the most demanding cognitive tasks in our everyday lives. Further questions naturally arise: how do children learn to master this demanding task, and what is the developmental trajectory in this domain? Research shows that aspects of turn-taking such as its timing are remarkably stable across languages and cultures, but the word order of languages varies enormously. How then does prediction of the incoming turn work when the verb (often the informational nugget in a clause) is at the end? Conversely, how can production work fast enough in languages that have the verb at the beginning, thereby requiring early planning of the whole clause? What happens when one changes modality, as in sign languages -- with the loss of channel constraints is turn-taking much freer? And what about face-to-face communication amongst hearing individuals -- do gestures, gaze, and other body behaviors facilitate turn-taking? One can also ask the phylogenetic question: how did such a system evolve? There seem to be parallels (analogies) in duetting bird species, and in a variety of monkey species, but there is little evidence of anything like this among the great apes. All this constitutes a neglected set of problems at the heart of the psychology of language and of the language sciences. This research topic welcomes contributions f ..." ;
    schema:exampleOfWork <http://worldcat.org/entity/work/id/4189268028> ;
    schema:name "Turn-Taking in Human Communicative Interaction" ;
    schema:productID "960769616" ;
    schema:publication <http://www.worldcat.org/title/-/oclc/960769616#PublicationEvent/s_l_frontiers_media_sa2016> ;
    schema:publisher <http://experiment.worldcat.org/entity/work/data/4189268028#Agent/frontiers_media_sa> ; # Frontiers Media SA
    schema:url <http://www.doabooks.org/doab?func=fulltext&rid=18212> ;
    schema:url <https://doi.org/10.3389/978-2-88919-825-2> ;
    schema:workExample <http://worldcat.org/isbn/9782889198252> ;
    wdrs:describedby <http://www.worldcat.org/title/-/oclc/960769616> ;
    .


Related Entities

<http://experiment.worldcat.org/entity/work/data/4189268028#Agent/frontiers_media_sa> # Frontiers Media SA
    a bgn:Agent ;
    schema:name "Frontiers Media SA" ;
    .

<http://experiment.worldcat.org/entity/work/data/4189268028#Person/casillas_marisa> # Marisa Casillas
    a schema:Person ;
    schema:familyName "Casillas" ;
    schema:givenName "Marisa" ;
    schema:name "Marisa Casillas" ;
    .

<http://experiment.worldcat.org/entity/work/data/4189268028#Person/holler_judith> # Judith Holler
    a schema:Person ;
    schema:familyName "Holler" ;
    schema:givenName "Judith" ;
    schema:name "Judith Holler" ;
    .

<http://experiment.worldcat.org/entity/work/data/4189268028#Person/kendrick_h_kobin> # Kobin Kendrick H.
    a schema:Person ;
    schema:familyName "Kendrick H." ;
    schema:givenName "Kobin" ;
    schema:name "Kobin Kendrick H." ;
    .

<http://experiment.worldcat.org/entity/work/data/4189268028#Person/levinson_c_stephen> # Stephen Levinson C.
    a schema:Person ;
    schema:familyName "Levinson C." ;
    schema:givenName "Stephen" ;
    schema:name "Stephen Levinson C." ;
    .

<http://worldcat.org/isbn/9782889198252>
    a schema:ProductModel ;
    schema:isbn "2889198251" ;
    schema:isbn "9782889198252" ;
    .

<http://www.doabooks.org/doab?func=fulltext&rid=18212>
    rdfs:comment "Description of rights in Directory of Open Access Books (DOAB): Attribution (CC by)" ;
    .


Content-negotiable representations

Close Window

Please sign in to WorldCat 

Don't have an account? You can easily create a free account.